What is Selective Attention in Psychology?

Selective attention, or the ability to concentrate on a stimulus, is a vital skill in our everyday lives. It helps us to avoid distractions and enhance relevant information. While selective attention is a basic cognitive process, it can be easily honed for better performance. There are two main types of selective attention: arousal-focused and discriminating focus.

Selective attention is a critical skill in many academic domains. It has been linked to literacy skills, mathematics, and language, among others. However, its effects on these domains are not uniform. Children may have varying strengths and weaknesses with selective attention. For example, children may need more cues to selectively attend, and some children may have difficulty selecting particular messages in the presence of competing stimuli.

The neural bases of selective attention have been identified. This is an important area of research. Selective attention is a multi-stage process, which influences the neural circuits supporting reading, math, and language. Studies have shown similar patterns in both children and adults, as well as in different socioeconomic groups. These findings provide a valuable framework for studying the foundations of academic achievement during development.

A key finding from this research was that a fronto-parietal network was engaged in selective attention, particularly to manage response conflict. In addition to the fronto-parietal network, additional regions of the anterior frontal gyrus were engaged.

One way to look at the neural bases of selective attention is through event-related brain potentials (ERPs). ERPs are electrical recordings of the brain’s response to a stimulus. During the first few hundred milliseconds after a stimulus onset, the brain modulates its activity to filter out task-relevant information. The effect of selective attention on early neural processing can be quite dramatic.

Researchers have investigated selective attention in both children and adults. Early experiments used dichotic listening, where participants were presented with one message and a second message at the same time. Participants were then told to repeat the first message back. Those who repeated the first message showed a greater shift in electrical activity than those who did not. They also showed larger N1s in the attended ear.

Early studies attempted to replicate a cocktail party scenario in a controlled laboratory environment. The researchers had the participants seated and asked them to listen to the two stimuli. Some of the participants were unaware of the stimuli, while the other participants were not. Either way, their ERPs were recorded in the attended or unattended channels. Typically, those who were not paying attention showed a wider negative ERP distribution. Among the probe stimuli, standard tones elicited larger N1s, while high-frequency tones elicited smaller N1s.

Another line of research has found that selective attention is enhanced when a child is trained to attend to the stimuli. Although the mechanism is not clear, training appears to help children improve their attention skills across a variety of domains. Likewise, a recent study found that selective attention could be strengthened with mind-body training.

Research on attention has also shown that it can be a force multiplier. Using this concept, it is possible to develop training programs that would help children increase their abilities to perform academic tasks.

Did you miss our previous article…
https://quietmeditations.com/power-nap-benefits-and-how-to-take-power-nap-meditation/

Author

Recommended For You

About the Author: James Quinto

James is a content creator who works in the personal development niche.